Microsoft Word turney-littman-acm doc



Yüklə 200 Kb.
Pdf görüntüsü
səhifə14/18
tarix22.05.2023
ölçüsü200 Kb.
#119806
1   ...   10   11   12   13   14   15   16   17   18
ra
c
y
SO-LSA
SO-PMI
Figure 13. Comparison of SO-LSA and SO-PMI with the GI lexicon and TASA. 
5.7. Varying the Number of Dimensions 
The behaviour of LSA is known to be sensitive to the number of dimensions of the matrix 
(the parameter k in Section 3.2). In this section, we investigate the effect of varying the 
number of dimensions for SO-LSA with the TASA corpus and the GI lexicon. Figure 14 
shows the accuracy of SO-LSA as a function of the number of dimensions. The k 
parameter varies from 50 to 300 dimensions, in increments of 50. The highest accuracy is 
achieved with 250 dimensions. Second highest is 200 dimensions, followed by 300 
dimensions. The graph suggests that the optimal value of k, for using SO-LSA with the 
TASA corpus, is somewhere between 200 and 300 dimensions, likely near 250 
dimensions. 


29
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
0
50
100
150
200
250
300
350
Number of Dimensions
A
c
c
u
ra
c
y
100% Threshold
75% Threshold
50% Threshold
25% Threshold
Figure 14. The effect of varying the number of dimensions for SO-LSA. 
5.8. Varying the Paradigm Words 
The standard methodology for supervised learning is to randomly split the labeled data 
(the lexicon, in this context) into a training set and a testing set. The sizes of the training 
and testing sets are usually approximately the same, within an order of magnitude. We 
think of SO-A as an unsupervised learning method, because the “training” set is only 
fourteen words (two orders of magnitude smaller than the testing set) and because the 
paradigm words were carefully chosen instead of randomly selected (defining rather than 
training). 
The fourteen paradigm words were chosen as prototypes or ideal examples of positive 
and negative semantic orientation (see Section 3). All fourteen paradigm words appear in 
the General Inquirer lexicon. The positive paradigm words are all tagged “Positiv” and 
the negative paradigm words are all tagged “Negativ” (although they were chosen before 
consulting the General Inquirer lexicon). As we mentioned, the paradigm words were 
removed from the testing words for our experiments. 
The following experiment examines the behaviour of SO-A when the paradigm words 
are randomly selected. Since rare words would tend to require a larger corpus for SO-A 
to work well, we controlled for frequency effects. For each original paradigm word, we 
found the word in the General Inquirer lexicon with the same tag (“Positiv” or “Negativ”) 
and the most similar frequency. The frequency was measured by the number of hits in 
AltaVista. Table 8 shows the resulting new paradigm words. 


30
Table 8. Original paradigm words and corresponding frequency-matched new 
paradigm words. 
Original 
paradigm word 
Frequency of 
original word 
Matched 
new word 
Frequency 
of new word 
Semantic 
orientation 
good 
55,289,359
right 
55,321,211
positive 
nice 
12,259,779
worth 
12,242,455
positive 
excellent 
11,119,032
commission 
11,124,607
positive 
positive 
9,963,557
classic 
9,969,619
positive 
fortunate 
1,049,242
devote 
1,052,922
positive 
correct 
11,316,975
super 
11,321,807
positive 
superior 
5,335,487
confidence 
5,344,805
positive 
bad 
18,577,687
lost 
17,962,401
negative 
nasty 
2,273,977
burden 
2,267,307
negative 
poor 
9,622,080
pick 
9,660,275
negative 
negative 
5,896,695
raise 
5,885,800
negative 
unfortunate 
987,942
guilt 
989,363
negative 
wrong 
12,048,581
capital 
11,721,649
negative 
inferior 
1,013,356
blur 
1,011,693
negative 
The inclusion of some of the words in Table 8, such as “pick”, “raise”, and “capital”, 
may seem surprising. These words are only negative in certain contexts, such as “pick on 
your brother”, “raise a protest”, and “capital offense”. We hypothesized that the poor 
performance of the new paradigm words was (at least partly) due to their sensitivity to 
context, in contrast to the original paradigm words. To test this hypothesis, we asked 25 
people to rate the 28 words in Table 8, using the following scale: 
1 = negative semantic orientation (in almost all contexts) 
2 = negative semantic orientation (in typical contexts) 
3 = neutral or context-dependent semantic orientation 
4 = positive semantic orientation (in typical contexts) 
5 = positive semantic orientation (in almost all contexts) 
Each person was given a different random permutation of the 28 words, to control for 
ordering effects. The average pairwise correlation between subjects’ ratings was 0.86. 
The original paradigm words had average ratings of 4.5 for the seven positive words and 
1.4 for the seven negative words. The new paradigm words had average ratings of 3.9 for 
positive and 2.4 for negative. These judgments lend support to the hypothesis that context 
sensitivity is higher for the new paradigm words; context independence is higher for the 


31
original paradigm words. On an individual basis, subjects judged the original word more 
context independent than the corresponding new paradigm word in 61% of cases 
(statistically significant, p < .01). 
To evaluate the fourteen new paradigm words, we removed them from the set of 
3,596 testing words and substituted the original paradigm words in their place. Figure 15 
compares the accuracy of the original paradigm words with the new words, using 
SO-PMI with AV-ENG and GI, and Figure 16 uses AV-CA. It is clear that the original 
words perform much better than the new words. 
Figure 17 and Figure 18 compare SO-PMI and SO-LSA on the TASA-ALL corpus 
with the original and new paradigm words. Again, the original words perform much 
better than the new words.
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100

Yüklə 200 Kb.

Dostları ilə paylaş:
1   ...   10   11   12   13   14   15   16   17   18




Verilənlər bazası müəlliflik hüququ ilə müdafiə olunur ©www.azkurs.org 2024
rəhbərliyinə müraciət

gir | qeydiyyatdan keç
    Ana səhifə


yükləyin